THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN MOTOR ACTIVITY AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN THE TEACHING-LEARNING PROCESS: CASE OF 6 - 7 AGED TUNISIAN PUPILS

Mounira Ben Sidhom, Nizar Souissi

Abstract


Considering that the cognitive and motor dimensions of human beings grow together and that primary school age is one of the most important stages of children’s cognitive and motor development, the aim of this study was to investigate the relationship between executive functions and motor activity in the teaching-learning process. Primary school students (N = 40; 6.3 years) participated in this study. They were divided into two groups: an experimental group (20 students) and a control group (20 students). The first group followed a 12-week motors activity program with 5 sessions of 40 minutes per week and the second a regular math sessions program. The results of the experimental group show a clear improvement in most of the study variables. Also, these results highlight the value of actively involving the student in the task and the importance of using active methods in the teaching-learning process.

 

Article visualizations:

Hit counter


Keywords


math class, children, motricity, working memory, inhibitory control

Full Text:

PDF

References


Abdelkarim, O., Ammar, A., Chtourou, H., Wagner, M., Knisel, E., Hökelmann, A., & Bös, K. (2017). Relationship between motor and cognitive learning abilities among primary school-aged children. Alexandria Journal of Medicine, 53(4), 325-331.

Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 71-82.

Aye, T., Oo, K. S., Khin, M. T., Kuramoto-Ahuja, T., & Maruyama, H. (2017). Reliability of the test of gross motor development second edition (TGMD-2) for Kindergarten children in Myanmar. Journal of Physical Therapy Science, 29(10), 1726-1731.

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F., . . Faraone, S. V. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 72(5), 757.

Clark, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2002). The relationship between executive function abilities, adaptive behaviour, and academic achievement in children with externalising behaviour problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(6), 785-796.

Cosnefroy, L. (2010). L’apprentissage autorégulé: perspectives en formation d’adultes. Savoirs(1), 9-50.

Cowan, N., AuBuchon, A. M., Gilchrist, A. L., Ricker, T. J., & Saults, J. S. (2011). Age differences in visual working memory capacity: Not based on encoding limitations. Developmental science, 14(5), 1066-1074.

Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037-2078.

De Lièvre, B., & Staes, L. (2000). La psychomotricité au service de l'enfant: notions et applications pédagogiques: De Boeck.

Diamond, A. (1995). Evidence of robust recognition memory early in life even when assessed by reaching behavior. Journal of experimental child psychology, 59(3), 419-456.

Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child development, 71(1), 44-56.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135-168.

Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget's AB task: Evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental brain research, 74, 24-40.

Ellis, L. K., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Individual differences in executive attention predict self‐regulation and adolescent psychosocial behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021(1), 337-340.

Ferguson, H. J., Brunsdon, V. E., & Bradford, E. E. (2021). The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Scientific reports, 11(1), 1382.

Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental psychology, 40(2), 177.

Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: two new school readiness indicators. Developmental psychology, 46(5), 1008.

Hale, S., Bronik, M. D., & Fry, A. F. (1997). Verbal and spatial working memory in school-age children: developmental differences in susceptibility to interference. Developmental psychology, 33(2), 364.

Hanakawa, T. (2011). Rostral premotor cortex as a gateway between motor and cognitive networks. Neuroscience research, 70(2), 144-154.

Haywood, K. M., & Getchell, N. (2001). Learning activities for life span motor development. (No Title).

Hillman, C. H., Castelli, D. M., & Buck, S. M. (2005). Aerobic fitness and neurocognitive function in healthy preadolescent children. Medicine and science in sports and exercise, 37(11), 1967.

Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Castelli, D. M., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Scudder, M. R., Kamijo, K. (2014). Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics, 134(4), e1063-e1071.

Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2017-2036.

Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., O’Leary, K. C., Scudder, M. R., Wu, C. T., Castelli, D. M., & Hillman, C. H. (2011). The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Developmental science, 14(5), 1046-1058.

Kulinna, P. H., Stylianou, M., Dyson, B., Banville, D., Dryden, C., & Colby, R. (2018). The effect of an authentic acute physical education session of dance on elementary students’ selective attention. BioMed Research International, 2018.

Libertus, K., & Hauf, P. (2017). Motor skills and their foundational role for perceptual, social, and cognitive development (Vol. 8, pp. 301): Frontiers Media SA.

McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2012). Self‐regulation in early childhood: Improving conceptual clarity and developing ecologically valid measures. Child development perspectives, 6(2), 136-142.

McKenna, R., Rushe, T., & Woodcock, K. A. (2017). Informing the structure of executive function in children: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 11, 154.

Metcalf, B. S., Hosking, J., Jeffery, A., Voss, L., Henley, W., & Wilkin, T. (2011). Fatness leads to inactivity, but inactivity does not lead to fatness: a longitudinal study in children (EarlyBird 45). Archives of disease in childhood, 96(10), 942-947.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41(1), 49-100.

Monette, S., & Bigras, M. (2008). La mesure des fonctions exécutives chez les enfants d'âge préscolaire. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 49(4), 323.

Muhoozi, G. K., Atukunda, P., Diep, L. M., Mwadime, R., Kaaya, A. N., Skaare, A. B., . . . Iversen, P. O. (2018). Nutrition, hygiene, and stimulation education to improve growth, cognitive, language, and motor development among infants in Uganda: A cluster‐randomized trial. Maternal & child nutrition, 14(2), e12527.

Myer, G. D., Faigenbaum, A. D., Edwards, N. M., Clark, J. F., Best, T. M., & Sallis, R. E. (2015). Sixty minutes of what? A developing brain perspective for activating children with an integrative exercise approach. British journal of sports medicine, 49(23), 1510-1516.

Pailhous, J., & Bonnard, M. (1989). Programmation et contrôle du mouvement. Traité de psychologie cognitive, 1.

Payne, V. G., & Isaacs, L. D. (2017). Human motor development: A lifespan approach: Routledge.

Petrican, R., Taylor, M. J., & Grady, C. L. (2017). Trajectories of brain system maturation from childhood to older adulthood: implications for lifespan cognitive functioning. Neuroimage, 163, 125-149.

Piaget, J. (1947). Psychologie de l'Intelligence Armand-Colin.

Piaget, J., & Cook, M. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children (p. 419). New York, NY, US: WW Norton & Co.

posner, M., & Rothbart, M. (1998). Attention, self–regulation and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 353(1377), 1915-1927.

Raver, C. C., Jones, S. M., Li‐Grining, C., Zhai, F., Bub, K., & Pressler, E. (2011). CSRP’s impact on low‐income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: self‐regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child development, 82(1), 362-378.

Reader, M. J., Harris, E. L., Schuerholz, L. J., & Denckla, M. B. (1994). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and executive dysfunction. Developmental neuropsychology, 10(4), 493-512.

Rigal, R. (1996). Motricité humaine: fondements et applications pédagogiques. 2, Développementmoteur: Presses universitaires du Québec.

Rudd, J., Buszard, T., Spittle, S., O'Callaghan, L., & Oppici, L. (2021). Comparing the efficacy (RCT) of learning a dance choreography and practicing creative dance on improving executive functions and motor competence in 6–7 years old children. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 53, 101846.

Rutman, A. M., Clapp, W. C., Chadick, J. Z., & Gazzaley, A. (2010). Early top–down control of visual processing predicts working memory performance. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 22(6), 1224-1234.

Sibley, B. A., & Etnier, J. L. (2003). The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: a meta-analysis. Pediatric exercise science, 15(3), 243-256.

Solesio-Jofre, E., Lorenzo-López, L., Gutiérrez, R., López-Frutos, J. M., Ruiz-Vargas, J. M., & Maestú, F. (2012). Age-related effects in working memory recognition modulated by retroactive interference. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67(6), 565-572.

Stevens, M. C., Skudlarski, P., Pearlson, G. D., & Calhoun, V. D. (2009). Age-related cognitive gains are mediated by the effects of white matter development on brain network integration. Neuroimage, 48(4), 738-746.

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2013). Brain structures associated with executive functions during everyday events in a non-clinical sample. Brain Structure and Function, 218, 1017-1032.

Tapert, S. F., Baratta, M. V., Abrantes, A. M., & Brown, S. A. (2002). Attention dysfunction predicts substance involvement in community youths. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(6), 680-686.

Ursache, A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The promotion of self‐regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achievement in children at risk for school failure. Child development perspectives, 6(2), 122-128.

van der Fels, I. M., de Bruijn, A., Renken, R., Königs, M., Meijer, A., Oosterlaan, J., . . . Smith, J. (2020). Relationships between gross motor skills, cardiovascular fitness, and visuospatial working memory-related brain activation in 8-to 10-year-old children. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20, 842-858.

Van der Fels, I. M., Te Wierike, S. C., Hartman, E., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Smith, J., & Visscher, C. (2015). The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4–16 year old typically developing children: A systematic review. Journal of science and medicine in sport, 18(6), 697-703.

Vandenbroucke, L., Spilt, J., Verschueren, K., Piccinin, C., & Baeyens, D. (2018). The classroom as a developmental context for cognitive development: A meta-analysis on the importance of teacher–student interactions for children’s executive functions. Review of Educational Research, 88(1), 125-164.

Veldman, S. L., Santos, R., Jones, R. A., Sousa-Sá, E., & Okely, A. D. (2019). Associations between gross motor skills and cognitive development in toddlers. Early human development, 132, 39-44.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). (No Title).

Yetti, E., Syarah, E. S., Pramitasari, M., Syarfina, S., & Susanti, D. (2019). The influence of the dance creativity on executive functions of early childhood. Paper presented at the International Conference on Arts and Design Education (ICADE 2018).

Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child development perspectives, 6(4), 354-360.




DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.46827/ejes.v10i10.5052

Refbacks

  • There are currently no refbacks.


Copyright (c) 2023 Mounira Ben Sidhom, Nizar Souissi

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Copyright © 2015-2023. European Journal of Education Studies (ISSN 2501 - 1111) is a registered trademark of Open Access Publishing Group. All rights reserved.


This journal is a serial publication uniquely identified by an International Standard Serial Number (ISSN) serial number certificate issued by Romanian National Library (Biblioteca Nationala a Romaniei). All the research works are uniquely identified by a CrossRef DOI digital object identifier supplied by indexing and repository platforms. All authors who send their manuscripts to this journal and whose articles are published on this journal retain full copyright of their articles. All the research works published on this journal are meeting the Open Access Publishing requirements and can be freely accessed, shared, modified, distributed and used in educational, commercial and non-commercial purposes under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (CC BY 4.0).